“The role of religion in Mozambican human values: a case study”

 

Costa, P.R.

patriciacosta@yahoo.com

ISCTE, Portugal

 

 

 

Introduction:

 

Studies have repeatedly demonstrated an existing relation between value priorities and a multiplicity of attitudes and behaviours, including religiosity (Barnea & Schwartz, 1994). Religion exerts a great influence on our social environment, and is important as a powerful social movement itself. For example, religion as been considered more advanced than science, also a social movement, as it motivates people more effectively (Adler, 1998). Social harmony of heterogonous religious societies presupposes a foundation of shared values. Still, not all values are compatible. Indeed, if there are conflicting values within a religion, most notably, there are antagonistic values between different religions. Because religions often involve undisputable authority and a single truth, such value differences express tensions that may erupt in an open conflict (Riis, 1999). The new global framework is challenging the religious identity and the status of religion in many regions of the world and changing previous normative standards into prescribed irregularities. Thus, at a time when religiously based conflict is more common now than at any time since the mid-1600s (Spickard, 1999), understanding the role played by religion in human values is extremely important as people become increasingly aware of the existence of multiple worldviews.

Mozambique is a country of 800,000 km², with Portuguese as the official language, which lends unity to a country formed by sixteen language groups with their respective tongues. In 1997, it had 17 million inhabitants, 75% of them less than fifteen years of age. Fittingly, Mozambique is characterised by a very heterogeneous population: the approximate rate of illiteracy is 78%; only a minority speaks the national language, and although 99% of the population belong to indigenous ethnic groups, and 50% maintain indigenous beliefs, the existent 20% of Muslim population is considered quite influential (Frye, 2001).

Evangelization in Mozambique developed along Portuguese colonization after the fifteenth century. While the tribes in Mozambique practiced their traditional rites and cults to the ancestors, the Portuguese desired to spread Catholic faith and so were accompanied by priests to lend spiritual assistance and celebrate the sacraments. Islamism was, at that time, the religion of a commercial minority situated in the coast. During these five centuries there were various attempts at evangelization, which ended with political independence and the adoption of a Marxist orientation. After Independence in 1975, missionaries were expelled and returned to their countries, seminarians were "nationalized" and young students were sent to be educated according to the socialist spirit, and 90% of the Catholics are said to have abandoned their faith (Velo, 1999). Nevertheless, a civil war would follow and it was not until 1992 that government and guerillas signed for peace. Currently, the south of Mozambique has a small Muslim population compared to the north of the country. “Their attitudes are a concern but [in the south] they lack demographic weight. On the contrary for the last five centuries on the northern coast almost all the inhabitants belong by tradition to Islam. Although they do not practice their religion, they were born in an Islamic tradition and consider themselves Muslims” (Velo, 1999: 3).  According to Velo, previous collaboration between Christians and Muslims ended with the end of the civil war. At that time, The African Muslim Agency is meant to have started an intense campaign of Islamic propaganda, which, according to the author, ended in an increasingly Islamic influence present in a higher number of mosques, means of social communication, assiduity in prayer, and Islamic feasts.

The study of the relation between social values and religion is quite an important one. Values are imparted through everyday exposure to costumes, laws, norms, scripts, and practices (Markus & Kitayama, 1994), which includes exposure to religion. For example, a society where success is highly valued is likely to have a competitive economic system, whereas a culture that emphasises group well being is likely to be characterized by more co-operative economic and legal systems. Consequently, the dynamic of values influences social behaviour by coordinating individual behaviour and social interaction. Then, we can say that religion plays a role in influencing social life by influencing individuals’ needs for social interaction, which certainly differs from religion to religion, and, simultaneously, is influenced by these values as it involves behaving in accordance with them. At a time where globalisation involves the universalisation of values and consequent behaviours, with applicable international laws, as is the case, for example, of human rights, development goals, or war on terrorism, issues of religion and values became a current, often polemic, debate. In fact, the issue of globalization has raised many discussions on the relation between human values and religion as, while some defend that certain human values should be universal, others defend that human values just express specific worldviews (Spickard, 1999).

For the purpose of this article, "religion" will refer to a set of beliefs and values pertaining to a “socially-affirmed worldview” and to a respective set of goals to pursue (Spickard, 1999) and assume that different relations between values associated with religion cannot produce superior or inferior worldviews. The paper shall now approach differences in the literature between Islamic and Christian values. On the differences between Islamic and Christian values, some have argued that Christian societies see individuals as being ‘above’ the collective, while Islam protects the person by emphasising the collective need to maintain a just society (Donnelly, 1982). In other words, traditional Islam puts the collective above the individual. Although traditional Muslim thought is often associated with anti-individualism, this is not the same to say that Islam values undermine the participation of individuals in social life. On the contrary, social interconnectedness finds a good expression in the respective religious values of social order, where people are supposed to take care, and responsibility for each other, equality of access to work, education, and to cultural affairs. Thus, one can assume that, within a diverse society, as is the case of Mozambique, religion plays a definite role in the organization of values, though the principal of organization might not be as clear and direct as individual versus collective oriented values. The paper will now take a deeper look at values theories in order to expound how have values generally been looked at and how might that help us understanding the role of religion in organizing these values in the Mozambican society.

The idea of Individualism and Collectivism is not a new one (see Schwartz and Bilsky, 1990). Largely, individualism and collectivism represent the opposite between caring for you versus being cared for by in-group members (e.g., Hofstede, 1980; Hui & Triandis, 1986; Triandis, 1989). Dynamically, individualism is related to urbanised, industrialised, and wealthy societies, while collectivism is related to traditional, agricultural societies (Singelis et al., 1995). In fact, research suggests (Topalova, 1997; Goodwin, 1998) that this social pattern will still apply even if values are submitted to sudden changes (e.g. the formerly Communist Bulgaria). Generally, researchers would view East Asian cultures as Collectivistic and Western cultures (with U.S.A. leading) as individualistic (e.g., Kim et al., 1994; Markus & Kitayama, 1994; Triandis, 1990). These cultural dimensions are supposed to emerge from specific cultural circumstances, namely, religion (Kim et. al., 1994). It is noteworthy that this distinction has actually benefited from wide evidence and supportive findings were found across numerous nations.

Recently, varied critiques have been raised to the individualism-collectivism dichotomy (e.g. Kagitcibasi, 1994; Schwartz, 1990, 1994). A frequent criticism is that the concepts derive from western research and therefore the items used may have different meanings in the different cultures (Moghaddam, 1987; cf. Yang & Yue, 1988). Furthermore, it misinterprets certain values by focusing on personal versus in-group interests, because certain values serve both interests (e.g., wisdom, mature love). For example, a re-examination (Schwartz, 1990) of Rokeach’s (1973) findings ingroup differences showed that it was actually priests who gave greater importance to independent thought, choice, action and creativity, previously considered exclusive of individualistic concerns. Therefore, the results illustrate the effect of confounding variables other than the effect of the same variable (Fijneman et al. 1996; Schwartz, 1994)

However, despite criticisms, the Individualism-Collectivism dichotomy provided an insightful view to group differences. Schwartz (1990: 151) concluded, “This is not contend, however, that the Individualism-Collectivism dichotomy is without merit… [but] finer distinctions are needed to attain a sharper and more accurate understanding of cultural and individual differences”. Schwartz (1990) presented theoretical and empirical arguments for why this dichotomy should be replaced by newer conceptualisations of value dimensions. Schwartz’s proposal is far more complex and it embraces a wider index of values. According to Schwartz (1990), the concept of individualism vs. collectivism cultures should be replaced by the idea of individualistic versus collectivist values, which do not necessarily conflict, but might co-exist in society. Therefore, values are organized in a continuum, representing the shared motivational emphasis. This article shall adopt Schwartz’s definition of values.

Schwartz does not deny that individuals might have individualistic or collectivistic goals, but argues that these are incompatible. While individualistic values serve the interests of the individual, the collective values concern prosocial motivations, that is, promote the interests of others. Subsequently, multi-dimensional dimensions are needed in order to encapsulate the dimensionality of cultural differences (Schwartz and Bilsky, 1990). Schwartz & Bilsky (1987) define values as beliefs that presume inherent feelings, refer to desirable goals and to the behaviours that lead to the achievement of these goals. For example, being helpful probably reflects a belief in social equality. Therefore, values work as criteria for judging and selecting behaviour and are organized by importance so that priority is given to values that promote the most important goals. Schwartz mapped values into ten value types, reflecting common goals and requirements: power, achievement, hedonism, stimulation self-direction, benevolence, tradition, and conformity. He hypothesised that power, achievement, hedonism, stimulation and self-direction serve individual interests while tradition, conformity, security, universalism, and benevolence values serve collective interests. In accordance with these, Schwartz’ s (1990) results showed that more collectivistic societies, like Hong Kong and Spain, might favour achievement values.

Finally, four higher dimensions are proposed to organize the relationship among motivational value types, which are subsequently summarised by two basic, bipolar dimensions. Higher dimensions emerge from the compatibilities observed between values while basic dimensions emerge from the compatibilities between higher orders. The four higher dimensions are openness to change, conservation, self-enhancement, and self-transcendence. Openness to change represents the values that motivate one to follow one’s own intellectual and emotional interests in unpredictable and uncertain directions; this is necessarily in conflict with conservation, which represents the values regarding motivations to preserve the statues quo and the certainty which relationships with close others, institutions and traditions provide. This conflict symbolises one basic dimension. The dimension of self-enhancement reflects the values that enhance individuals’ own personal interests, even if it implies costs to others, and is in conflict with self-transcendence, which characterizes values in terms of motivations to transcend selfish concerns and promote the welfare of others, close and distant, and of nature. This conflict symbolises the second basic dimension. The value type hedonism is related both to openness to change and to self-enhancement.

Succinctly, the objectives of this study are: 1) to test whether, within one country, religiously different regions exhibit different values. 2) To assess how Schwartz’s theory can be useful in understanding religiously different approaches within one single cultural setting, and finally, 3) to evaluate whether a combination of Schwartz’s values with individual and collective values is possible or better approach given the context.

We hypothesise that 1) religion will be an important factor in values of different religiously different regions; 2) that Schwartz’s value types and higher dimensions will be significantly different in different religious regions, and 3) that individualistic versus collectivistic values will benefit understanding values in religiously different regions within one country. 


 

An adaptation of Schwartz’s (1994) value survey was used to assess values. The questionnaire comprised 30 items and respondents were asked to rate the degree of importance of a value “AS A GUIDING PRINCIPLE IN MY LIFE” on a 5-point scale (from opposed to my values to extremely important to me).

 

Participants were 144 subjects, 71 youngsters, and 73 adults. 49% of the population lived in a Muslim province and 50.7% in a Christian province.

 

 

 

Variables

 

Independent variables were religion, age group, and gender. Age group and gender were used as control variables in order to guarantee that these had no effect on results. Dependent variables were values (single, organised in value types and in value dimensions). 


 

Results

 

Univariate Analysis of Variance looked at results. When looking at the values’ rating one can see relevant differences at different levels. The significant differences found are as follows:

 

Table 1: Analysis of effects of religion, gender, and age group

 

 

These results indicate that there was a significant effect of religion (F (1,136) = 2.961, p<.001). There was no significant effect of gender (F (1,136) = 1.367, p = .244) or age group (F (1,136) = 2.696, p = .103). There was no significant interaction between any of the variables. Thus, we conclude that both gender and age group are not independent variables in this study.

 

Furthermore, these results support our first hypothesis that religion is an important factor in values of different religiously different regions

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

The following tables present means and reports ANOVA findings for Schwartz’s value types, that is, values grouped by common goals and requirements.[1]

 

Table 2: One-way between-subjects ANOVA results for Schwartz’s value types

As one can see, four value types (achievement (F (1,142) = 10.188, p = .002), universalism (F (1,142) = 5.586, p = .019), benevolence (F (1,142) = 6.670, p = .011), and tradition (F (1,142) = 8.184, p = .005)) present significant differences between groups and one other (self-direction (F (1,142) = 3.369, p = .069)) is tendentiously different between groups. Thus, these findings support our second hypothesis which postulated that Schwartz’s value types would be significantly different in different religious regions. Next, we will look at mean results for value types in order to understand the direction of the difference.

 

Table 3: Mean results for Schwartz’s 10 value types

These results suggest that value types that are significantly different or tendentiously significantly different  between religious groups, all have a higher mean in the Christian group (achievement (M Christian=4.38, M Muslim=3.83; universalism (M Christian=3.91, M Muslim = 3.71, benevolence M Christian=3.53, M Muslim = 3.61, and tradition (M Christian =4.09, M Muslim = 3.81))

 

However, in this study, these ten value types have not been reproduced as an Cronbach's Alpha Reliability analysis showed value types had no internal consistency (self-direction µ = .2686; universalism µ = .4948; benevolence µ = .5707; tradition µ= .2156; security µ = .3998). [2]

 

Nevertheless, as values are organized in a continuum, results have been analyzed on the light of Schwartz’s four higher dimensions since these include all the values included in value types but with a more flexible organization has values within higher dimensions do not have to respect the organization of value types. The following table shows ANOVA results for Schwartz’s four higher dimensions.

 

Table 4: ANOVA results for Schwartz’s four higher dimensions

 

Thus, findings indicate that there are significant differences concerning three out four Schwartz’s higher dimensions (self-enhancement (F (1,142) = 4.296, p = .040); self-transcendence (F (1,142) = 9.021, p = .003); conservation (F (1,142) = 7.546, p = .007)) and there is a tendency for the fourth dimension to be significant (openness to change (F (1,142) = 3.369, p = .069)). Next, we shall look at mean results in order to understand what the direction of these differences is.

 

Table 5: Mean results for Schwartz’s four higher dimensions

 

As there seems to be a tendency for the Christian group to always rate values higher, a graph of the different ratings was done in order to better understand results. Because of this tendency, the extremes of the scale have not been considered for analysis.

 


Graph 1: rating of Schwartz’s 4 higher dimensions by the Christian group, in percentage

 

 

Graph 2: rating of Schwartz’s 4 higher dimensions by the Muslim group, in percentage

 


To recall, openness to change represents the values that motivate one to follow one’s own interests; conservation represents values regarding motivations to preserve relationships with others; self-enhancement reflects values that enhance individuals’ own personal interests, even if it implies costs to others, and self-transcendence characterizes values in terms of motivations to transcend selfish concerns and promote the welfare of others. Succinctly, openness to change and self-enhancement focus on the self, and self-transcendence and conservation on the preservation of the relation to others.

 

In the graphs above, we can observe that, there is a higher percentage of the Christian group rating conservation (77.6%) and self-transcendence (77.6%) values as “very important” when compared to the Muslim group (respectively, 68.6% and 60.4%). However, the Muslim group has higher ratings of the same value dimensions when rating them as “important” (23.3% in conservation and 33.7% in self-transcendence versus 8.6% and 17.2% for the respective values in the Christian group).

 

 

With regard to openness to change and self-enhancement dimensions, dimensions which concern the self, there seems to be a tendency for the Muslim group to consider these value dimensions as not important (5.8% and 7% respectively). The majority of the Christian group considers these two value dimensions as very important (53.4% for openness to change and 65.5% for self-enhancement). These value dimensions are also value by the Muslim group, although not by the majority, there is a tendency for rejecting self-transcendence, which includes the higher percentage so far of values “opposed to my values”. 

 

 

 

In order to test our third hypothesis, that individualistic versus collectivistic values will benefit understanding values in religiously different regions within one country, we have carried out another ANOVA test. Individualistic values compound the mean of power, achievement, hedonism, and self-direction; while collectivistic values concern the mean of conformity, security, tradition, universalism, and benevolence.

 

Table 6: ANOVA results for Schwartz’s individualistic versus collectivistic values

 

Here, we can see that when values are grouped into individual versus collective values, according to Schwartz’s distinction, they become significantly different between Christian and Muslim regions. Thus, both individualistic (F(1,142) = 7.999, p< .005) and collectivistic (F(1,142) = 10.014, p< .002) values are significantly different between religiously different regions.

 


Table 7: Mean results for Schwartz’s individualistic versus collectivistic values

As results show a tendency for the Christian region to rate higher most of the values, once again, the option was to produce a graph of value ratings.


 


 

Graph 3: rating of collectivistic and individualistic values by the Christian group, in percentage

 

Graph 4: rating of collectivistic and individualistic values by the Muslim group, in percentage


 

Thus, although the Christian group rate higher both values, one can see that the 63.8% of the Christian group rate individualistic values as “very important”, while only 37.2% of the Muslim group rate equally the same values. Furthermore, there is a higher percentage of people in the Muslim group rating individualistic values as “not important” (4.7%) compared to the same rating by the Christian group (1.7%). In addition, although there is 79.3% of Christian group rating collective values as “very important”, versus 71% in the Muslim group, overall, if we had the 12.1% of “important” values to the rating of collectivistic values by Christians, and the 26.7% equally rated by Muslims in the same values.

 

These results confirm our third hypothesis that individualistic and collectivistic values benefit understanding values in religiously different regions within one country. 

 


 


 

 

 

 

 

 

 


 

 

Conclusion

 

Generally, it seems possible to conclude that a general coherence in national values does not prevent individuals from differing in the overall strength of these values. In fact, religious contexts seem to be one important determinant of differences found in values. Accordingly, previous studies have found religious practice to be correlated with attitudes concerning both political viewpoints and proposed universal values (e.g. human rights) (in Spini & Doise, 1998). Furthermore, values differ in the relative efficacy people attribute to institutions (governmental and political parties) and to themselves for having these values respected (Doise et al., 1999). Thus, it seems plausible to argue that religious viewpoints can collide with values since the dynamics of religion in society (its respect by institutions, its general regional and national cohesion, its support, or lack of it, by the government, etc) most probably strongly contributes to differences found in religious positioning towards values.

Schwartz’s (1990) value types and dimensions have revealed important in understanding differences based on religion. Thus, these results suggest that Muslim and Christian populations differ in important values – namely, universalism, benevolence, achievement, and tradition - whereby Christians rate all the values higher. This result, further explored in the ratings presentation by graphics, was interpreted as a generous use of the scale. However, it is also possible that these values do not represent important values for a Muslim population and therefore they have not identified with most of the values presented. In fact, this line of thought would follow suggestions that the universalisation of values derived from the Cold War when capitalist societies politically opposed, and considered better, ‘their’ values of freedom and individual achievement to the values of economic and social equality, characteristic of the then Communist societies. Accordingly, some have argued that privileged modern values, which would certainly include definitions of values studied by science, would steam from Western philosophic principles and is thus not applicable to other civilisations: “Value systems, such as  ... freedom of the press, are choices which each society must make. In the United States, individuals … are placed ahead of law and order. In Asia, law and order have priority ... These are simply alternative value systems. None are superior over the other. Asians and Westerners must emancipate themselves from notions that Western value systems are superior …” (in Chew, 1994: 936).

Despite this, results indicate that Schwartz’s (1990) values are a useful tool in understanding value differences in religiously divergent populations. Thus, although only further research can elucidate  why religiously different populations have different values, this study has taken a step further in understanding existing differences as it shows that religion, and neither age group nor gender, is a determinant of different value systems. Furthermore, Schwartz’s four higher dimensions have revealed significant differences[3] in both groups. This suggests that the notions of motivation to follow one’s interests (openness to change); motivations to preserve relationships with others (conservation); enhancement of individuals’ own personal interests even at the cost of others (self-enhancement), and motivations to transcend selfish concerns and promote the welfare of others (self-transcendence) are important in the comprehension of religious approaches to values.

 

Furthermore, the distinction between Individualistic and Collectivistic values has proved a helpful tool in understanding group differences, as both have revealed significantly different between groups. Thus, although results indicate that the Christian group rates higher both individualistic and collectivistic values, the exploratory analysis of this rating indicates that further analysis should be taken in order to improve the comprehension of different motivations to these values. Still, these results are in line with previous research that found a link between Individualism/Collectivism and religion (Kim et. al., 1994). Along these lines, while criticisms to Individualism/Collectivism argued the items could have different meanings in different cultures (Moghaddam, 1987; cf. Yang & Yue, 1988), the fact that this research is carried in one nation, might approximate the meanings of the items, and that might become an added strength to using these concepts in this research, instead of the opposite.

 

This piece of research indicates that a combination of Schwartz’ values with the insights brought forward by the Individualism/ Collectivism proposal might create a powerful tool of analysis in certain contexts, where the simple analysis of universal values is not enough, especially if one is investigating differences within a single nation. Schwartz’s distinction between individual and collective values seems to add strength to his theory, which revealed important in the understanding of the role of religion in shaping human values. Overall, although many questions are now raised, it seems that many doors have also been open for forthcoming research. One improvement for future research is to investigate further personal views of the relation between religion and values. Thus, exploring differences in the relation between religious beliefs, practices, and communities of individuals and the surrounding society improves our comprehension of the ways in which historical, cultural and religious narratives influence values and beliefs. Accordingly, it seems most important to investigate further the historical and social influences that shape individual and communal identities with regard to questions of religion. Most importantly for present times, exploring the social influences in worldviews from a religious standpoint implies evaluating how dynamics of power and the demands of justice affect situations of conflicting interests and values. On reflection upon the actual global scenery, gaining substantial insight into the relationship between religious traditions and modern society, between religious values and contemporary values, ideas and beliefs, is not only a great contribution to research but also a necessary and crucial tool to the comprehension of the world we live in.

 

 

 

 

 

 


 

 

·       Adler, A. (1998). Social interest. (Colin Brett, Eds). New York: Oneworld Publications

·       Barnea, M.F.; Schwartz, S.H. (1994). Values and voting. Manuscript submitted for publication

·       Chew, M. (1994). Human Rights in Singapore. Asian Survey 34(11): 933-48.

·       Doise, W.; Spini, D.; Clémence, A. (1999). Human rights studied as social representations in a cross-cultural context. European Journal of Social Psychology,  29, 1 – 29

·       Doise, W.; Dell’Ambrogio, P., & Spini D. (1991). Psychologie sociale et Droits de l’Homme. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 4 (3/4), 257 – 277

·       Donnelly, J. (1982). Human Rights and Human Dignity: An Analytic Critique of Non-Western Conceptions of Human Rights. American Political Science Quarterly 76(2): 303-16.

·       Fijneman, Y.; Willemsen, M.E.; Poortinga, Y.H. et al. (1996). Individualism-collectivism an empirical study of a conceptual issue. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27, 381 – 402

·       Frye. (2001). Moçambique: ensaios. Revista engenho. Ed. UFRJ: Rio de Janeiro. www.funesc.com.br/engenho3/textos/ecul_to3.htm

·       Goodwin, R. (1998). Personal Relationships Across Cultures.  Routledge: London, UK

·       Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s Consequences, International Differences in Work-Related Values. Sage: London, UK

·       Hofstede, G. (1994). Cultures and Organizations: Software of the mind. Harper-Collins, London

·       Hogg, M.A.; Vaugham, G.M. (1998). Social Psychology (2nd ed). Prentice Hall Europe: Hertfordshire, UK

·       Hui, C.H.; Triandis, H.C. (1986). Individualism-collectivism: a study of cross-cultural researchers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 17, 225 – 248

·       Kagitcibasi, C. (1994). A critical appraisal of individualism and collectivism. In Kim, U.; Triandis, H.C.; Kagitcibasi, C.; Choi, S.-C.; Yong, G. (ed.). Individualism and collectivism: theory, method and applications (pp. 52 – 65). Sage, Thousand Oaks, CAKaplan, B.H.; Cassel, J.C.; Gore, S. (1977). Social support and health. Medical care, 15, 47 – 58

·       Kim, U.; Triandis, H.C.; Kagitcibasi, C.; Choi, S.-C.; Yong, G. (1994). Individualism and collectivism: theory, method and applications. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA

·       Markus, H.R.; Kitayama, S. (1994). A collective fear of the collective: implications for selves and theories of selves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 568 – 579

·       Moghaddam, F.M. (1987). Psychology in three worlds: as reflected by the crisis in social psychology and the move toward indigenous third-world psychology. American Psychologist, 42, 912 – 920

·       Riis, O. (1999). Modes of Religious Pluralism under Conditions of Globalisation". MOST Journal on Multicultural Societies, vol.1, no.1

·       Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. Free Press, New York

·       Schwartz, S.H. (1990). Individualism-Collectivism: Critique and Proposed Refinements. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, Vol. 21, No. 2, 139-157

·       Schwartz, S.H. (1992). Universals in the Content and Structure of Values: theoretical Advances and Empirical Tests in 20 Countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 25, pp.1 – 65

·       Schwartz, S. H. (1994a). Studying Human Values. In Bouvy, A.; Vijver, F.; Boski, P.; Schmitz, P. Journeys Into Cross-Cultural Psychology. (pp. 239 – 254). Swets & Zeitlinger: Amsterdam

·       Schwartz, S. H. (1994b). Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of Human values? Journal of Social Issues, Vol. 50, No., Vol. 50, No. 4, pp. 19-45

·       Schwartz, S. H.  (1997) Values and Culture. In Mumro, D.; Schumaker, J.F.; Carr, S.C. (pp.69-84). Motivation & Culture. Routledge: New York

·       Schwartz, S. H. (1999). Cultural Dimensions and Value Types. Workshop presented at the IACCP Conference, Graz, Austria, 1999

·       Schwartz, S. H.; Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward psychological structure of human values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol.53,  550-562

·       Schwartz, S. H.; Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a Theory of the Universal Content and structure of Values: Extensions and Cross-Cultural Replications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol.58, No.5, 878-891

·       Schwartz, S. H.; Bilsky, W. (1990b). Toward a Theory of the Universal Content and structure of Values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol.53, No.3, 550-562

·       Schwartz, S.H.; Huisman, S. (1995). Value priorities and religiosity in four Western religions. Social Psychology Quarterly, 58, 88 – 107

·       Schwartz, S.H.; Sagiv, L. (1995). Value Priorities and Readiness for Out-Group Social Contact. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 69, No. 3, 437 – 448

·       Singelis, T.M.; Triandis, H.C.; Bhawuk, D.S.; Gelfand, M. (1995). Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: a theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross-Cultural Research, 29, 240 – 275

·       Spickard, J.V. (1999). Human Rights, Religious Conflict, and Globalization. Ultimate Values in a New World Order. MOST Journal on Multicultural Societies, vol.1, no.1

·       Spini, D.; Doise, W. (1998). Organizing principles of involvement in human rights and their social anchoring in value priorities. European Journal of Social Psychology,  28, 603 – 622

·       Topalova, V. (1997). Individualism/Collectivism and social identity. Journal of Community and Applied Psychology, 7, 53 – 64

·       Triandis, H.C. (1989). The Self and Social Behaviour in Differing Cultural Contexts. Psychological Review, Vol. 96, No. 3, pp. 506 – 520

·       Triandis, H.C. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of individualism and collectivism. In Berman, J. (ed.) Nebraska symposium on motivation. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln

·       Triandis, H. C.(1995).  Individualism & Collectivism. Westview Press: London

·       Thompson, R.; Davies, A. (2000).  A study of parenting in the provinces of Maputo and Zambezia to determine the nature of a guide to parenting for basic education. The Pedagogical University.

·       Velo, M.(1999). http://www.famvin.org/cm/curia/vincentiana/1999/1999/VT994/VT994-18-IN-Velo.html.

·       Yang, K.S.; Yue, A.B. (1988). Social-oriented and individual-oriented achievement motives: conceptualisation and measurement. Paper presented at the XXIV International Congress of Psychology, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

 



[1] Tradition and conformity have been grouped, resulting of pre-tests, to form a new variable also denominated tradition as conformity included only the item ‘honouring parents and elders’[1], which seems to fit well the definition of tradition.

[2] The alpha of other value types were not presented because these were composed of only one item due to the lack of understanding in pre-tests developed.

[3] Three were statistically significant and the fourth was tendentiously significant